Thursday, September 27, 2007
Lexical knowledge
First a child learns to sound out words; then the child learns that when certain words are sounded out, they do not make sense. As we grow, and as we are exposed to more and more text, we learn new irregular words. As a child, you learned words like ONE, SHOE, and PEOPLE. As you read more, you learned words like CHOIR, COLONEL and ISLAND. Later still, you learned words like GEYSER, FEIGN, and BUREAU, and if you don't already know them, in the future, you may learn such words as SYNECDOCHE, BAREGE, and CACHET. In short, a person's ability to correctly read irregular words is directly related to their exposure to those words combined with information about the correct pronunciation (It is not uncommon for people to have read a word dozens of times, to know the meaning of that word, but to not know its correct pronunciation.).
Whereas a test of regular word reading (cipher knowledge) is strengthened by using words that the child is not familiar with, any test of irregular word reading (lexical knowledge) should use words that one could reasonably expect the child to be familiar with. This can be accomplished using leveled word lists, or by using words from previous vocabulary lessons, or by using words from the children's own leveled texts.
Once the words are chosen for an appropriate test of irregular word reading, the test can take one of several formats. The child can be asked to simply read the words aloud, or the child could be asked to find a word from a set that does not belong with the others (e.g. LINT, MINT, PINT). Similarly, the test might ask the student to find a rhyme for each regular word from a set (e.g. find the word that rhymes with CHOIR - CHAIR, CHORE, WIRE). Also, a test of irregular word reading could ask children to match pairs of words that have the same letter sounds (e.g. REIGN, SIGN, MINE).
Friday, September 21, 2007
About Online Casino
Who like to investing or playing game with Casino sites in your free time. There many Casino sites in the Internet some of them are Cheat. This Online Casino Blue Book can show you all of Casino sites by compare with rating or ranking of the month such as Top 10 Online casinos, Top 10 - No Deposit Casinos, Top 4 Online Bingo Casinos, Top 3 Online Poker Rooms and Top 5 Online Sportsbook Sites. All of these data can help you to choosing some online casino, online gambling or online blackjack sites for investing or Playing.
Most of online casino, online gambling, online blackjack .The Players will be able to play there number one Vegas style casino gambling games. Play craps, baccarat, blackjack, slots, poker, keno and bingo. These internet online casinos where hand selected based on these primary principles.
Online Casino Blue Book reviewed hundreds of casinos and decided to put them in a directory. Online Casino Blue Book directory lists only gambling casinos that have built a reputation on quality, customer service, and an overall superb online casino experience. Online Casino Blue Book site with top customer support, largest free casino bonuses, best loyalty bonus programs and best internet casino game payouts.
I think you can choose some of Online Casino sites from this site, but all of Investment have chancy, Good luck for all.
Wednesday, September 19, 2007
Elements Supporting Decoding
The relationship between the way a word is spelled and the way that word is pronounced in English is reasonably predictable. Certain conventions, collectively known as the English cipher, loosely govern English spelling and pronunciation. To be able to decode words, children need an implicit understanding of those conventions.
The best test of the child's ability to sound out regular words is to ask her to name isolated (out of context) words that are not already familiar to the child. Using unfamiliar words insures that the child is deciphering them, and not just recognizing them or guessing based on contextual cues. Some tests simply use real, regular words that are so rare that it is unlikely that the words are familiar to the child (e.g. PUN, MOCK, LOOT), but some tests use invented or made-up words (called pseudowords) to insure that the child does not have any prior experience with the test items (e.g. PARD, ORT, SERT). Some tests attempt to make the task more authentic by asking the child to read aloud a list of people's names (to pretend they are "calling roll"). The names are spelled phonetically and are not difficult to pronounce -- they can range from very easy names for young children (e.g. JIM WILLS, STAN HILL, etc.) to somewhat more challenging for older children (e.g. WANDA BOLTON, VICTOR CONRAD, etc.).
For young readers who are not quite able to sound out regular words, it is appropriate to test their basic letter-sound knowledge (which is a precursor to regular word reading). Common letter-sound knowledge assessments ask a child to identify a letter that could represent a speech sound (e.g. Identify the letter that makes the /s/ sound.), or ask the child to generate a sound or several sounds that could be represented by a letter (e.g. What sound(s) do(es) the letter "S" make?). In some assessments of letter-sound knowledge, the letters are embedded in the context of words, and the child is asked to identify the first letter (or sometimes the vowel sound) of a spoken word.
www.sedl.org
Monday, September 17, 2007
Lexical knowledge
First a child learns to sound out words; then the child learns that when certain words are sounded out, they do not make sense. As we grow, and as we are exposed to more and more text, we learn new irregular words. As a child, you learned words like ONE, SHOE, and PEOPLE. As you read more, you learned words like CHOIR, COLONEL and ISLAND. Later still, you learned words like GEYSER, FEIGN, and BUREAU, and if you don't already know them, in the future, you may learn such words as SYNECDOCHE, BAREGE, and CACHET. In short, a person's ability to correctly read irregular words is directly related to their exposure to those words combined with information about the correct pronunciation (It is not uncommon for people to have read a word dozens of times, to know the meaning of that word, but to not know its correct pronunciation.).
Whereas a test of regular word reading (cipher knowledge) is strengthened by using words that the child is not familiar with, any test of irregular word reading (lexical knowledge) should use words that one could reasonably expect the child to be familiar with. This can be accomplished using leveled word lists, or by using words from previous vocabulary lessons, or by using words from the children's own leveled texts. Once the words are chosen for an appropriate test of irregular word reading, the test can take one of several formats. The child can be asked to simply read the words aloud, or the child could be asked to find a word from a set that does not belong with the others (e.g. LINT, MINT, PINT). Similarly, the test might ask the student to find a rhyme for each regular word from a set (e.g. find the word that rhymes with CHOIR - CHAIR, CHORE, WIRE). Also, a test of irregular word reading could ask children to match pairs of words that have the same letter sounds (e.g. REIGN, SIGN, MINE).
Sunday, September 16, 2007
Elements Supporting Decoding
The relationship between the way a word is spelled and the way that word is pronounced in English is reasonably predictable. Certain conventions, collectively known as the English cipher, loosely govern English spelling and pronunciation. To be able to decode words, children need an implicit understanding of those conventions.
The best test of the child's ability to sound out regular words is to ask her to name isolated (out of context) words that are not already familiar to the child. Using unfamiliar words insures that the child is deciphering them, and not just recognizing them or guessing based on contextual cues. Some tests simply use real, regular words that are so rare that it is unlikely that the words are familiar to the child (e.g. PUN, MOCK, LOOT), but some tests use invented or made-up words (called pseudowords) to insure that the child does not have any prior experience with the test items (e.g. PARD, ORT, SERT). Some tests attempt to make the task more authentic by asking the child to read aloud a list of people's names (to pretend they are "calling roll"). The names are spelled phonetically and are not difficult to pronounce -- they can range from very easy names for young children (e.g. JIM WILLS, STAN HILL, etc.) to somewhat more challenging for older children (e.g. WANDA BOLTON, VICTOR CONRAD, etc.).
For young readers who are not quite able to sound out regular words, it is appropriate to test their basic letter-sound knowledge (which is a precursor to regular word reading). Common letter-sound knowledge assessments ask a child to identify a letter that could represent a speech sound (e.g. Identify the letter that makes the /s/ sound.), or ask the child to generate a sound or several sounds that could be represented by a letter (e.g. What sound(s) do(es) the letter "S" make?). In some assessments of letter-sound knowledge, the letters are embedded in the context of words, and the child is asked to identify the first letter (or sometimes the vowel sound) of a spoken word.
Sunday, September 9, 2007
The best plastic surgery in California
I would like to produce the best of plastic surgery in California. Especially in Los Angeles or Beverly Hills. There’re Rodeo Drive Plastic Surgeons; Rodeo Drive Liposuction, Rodeo Drive Rhinoplasty and Rodeo Drive Tummy Tuck.
At Rodeo Drive Plastic Surgeons provide you with exceptional surgical care - safely, professionally, and at a reasonable price. They offer breast augmentation, breast lift surgery, breast reduction, liposuction, tummy tuck surgery, facelift surgery, eyelid lifts, rhinoplasty or nose surgery and many other surgical and nonsurgical techniques. At Rodeo Drive Plastic Surgeons and advanced techniques have recently been featured on Access Hollywood, The Los Angeles Times, Los Angeles Magazine, Fortune Magazine, The Wall Street Journal, The New York Times, The Los Angeles Daily News, KTLA Morning News, Univision, Telemundo, Telefutura, and other media outlets.
Rodeo Drive Plastic Surgeons is also getting attention around the world. Some of their recent patients have traveled to us from Germany, New Zealand, Senegal, Saudi Arabia, Japan, Canada, New York, New Jersey, Massachusetts, Kansas, Alabama, Mississippi, Arizona, Nevada, Washington State, and Northern California.
Tummy tuck in Los Angeles
Tummy tuck is the one of the fastest-growing plastic surgery procedures. More and more people are deciding that a flat abdomen will enhance their well-being and self-esteem.
Rhinoplasty in Beverly Hills
Los Angeles plastic surgeons have exceptional academic backgrounds. Their work has received a great deal of press coverage in the media, both in magazines and on television. Dr. Lloyd Krieger is a plastic surgeon who focuses on creating an attractive nose during your rhinoplasty.
Liposuction in Los Angeles
Liposuction is the most common plastic surgery procedure. The reason is simple: The results tend to be excellent, and many of us have body areas which we wish were thinner.
Medical Director
Dr. Krieger founded Rodeo Drive Plastic Surgery to improve the care and experiences of plastic surgery patients. He maintains operating privileges at UCLA Medical Center, Cedars-Sinai Medical Center and Northridge Hospital.
Syntax
It is difficult to assess a child's syntactic knowledge without using printed text, but as was the case with semantics, if the child must process text to take the test, then the test becomes more of a decoding skills test than a test of syntax. It is possible to make some estimations about the child's productive syntactic knowledge by listening to the sentences that the child forms when she is talking, but teachers should be aware that the productive syntax may reflect the child's own dialect, which may follow non-standard syntactic rules, and may not reflect the child's ability to process and understand syntax in other dialects (it is common for people to speak one dialect, but be able to understand many other dialects).
A child can also be asked to identify sentences (spoken) that are syntactically incorrect (e.g. "Jane and her dog the hill they climbed it" or more subtly, "Me and Jane walked up the hill."), and additionally, the child can be asked to restructure the sentence correctly.
Another common syntax test involves presenting the child with sentences which have one word omitted, and asking the child to suggest words that could fill the blanks. In this case, the meaning the word is not what is being evaluated, but instead, the child is graded based upon the syntactic appropriateness of the word. So, for example, the child may fill in the sentence, "Mary fell off of the ___" with any of several obviously appropriate words such as "chair" "house" or "wagon," but credit should be given for any noun that the child supplies (e.g. "leaf" "brain" or "mop") because they are all syntactically correct.
Another assessment of syntax involves the child's ability to combine simple sentences into complex sentences, and to add modifiers appropriately. So for example, "Brownies taste good" could be combined with, "Mary likes to eat brownies" to make the complex sentence, "Mary likes to eat brownies because they taste good." Similarly, children could be asked to appropriately insert the words, "brown" "big" and "quickly" into the sentence, "The spider ran up the wall" to make a more complex sentence such as "The big, brown spider quickly ran up the wall."
Similarly, a child's syntax can be assessed through a test of her ability to change tense and modifiers of sentences. For example, a child could be asked to restructure, "I went to the store" to the future tense.
www.sedl.org